The role of declarative and procedural
knowledge in teaching foreign language grammar
Gabriela Lojová
Abstract:
The role of declarative and procedural knowledge in foreign
language teaching has been permanently discussed among theoreticians as well as
teacher practitioner and experts are searching for the optimal model of foreign
language teaching. In this article I introduce the adaptation of
Johnson´s model that enables educators to respect learners´
variables as well as current socio-cultural and educational conditions in
Slovakia. The explanation of the essential
characteristics of central concepts
(declarative and procedural knowledge, automization) from psychological
and methodological perspectives is
followed by a suggested learning sequence and some recommendations for teaching
practice based on psychology of learning.
Key words:
Foreign language
learning/teaching, declarative knowledge, procedural knowledge, atomization,
proceduralization,
For decades there have been
never-ending discussions among experts in foreign language (FL) learning and
teaching on various aspects and differences between language learning and
acquisition, explicit and implicit knowledge, focus on accuracy vs. fluency, the
role of knowledge and skills (DeKeyser 1997, Ellis 1997, MacWinning
2004,Rutherford and Scharwood Smith 1988 et al). Experts, in their endeavour to
provide teachers and methodologists with sound theoretical background
underlying various teaching methods, search for more effective approaches and models
of FL teaching. In so doing, it is of vital importance to realize that to find
the most suitable model they have to bear in mind not only various linguistic and
psychological aspects of FL teaching processes but also various internal and
environmental components determining the process of FL learning. In this
article I would like to focus on the role of declarative and procedural
knowledge in foreign language grammar teaching to secondary school learners.
Last two decades in Slovak
education, as well as in other surrounding countries, have been characterized
by many changes in foreign language teaching; the permanent re-evaluation of
our teaching practice, efforts to implement newly introduced teaching
approaches, methods and techniques, in order to make language learning more
efficient and to meet the changing needs of our learners. In spite of all the
changes our experience still shows that secondary school students achieve a
relatively high level of knowledge about the language, but their ability to
apply this knowledge in real communication is very often much inferior. Common
practice shows that students can learn grammar rules and structures easily and
are able to use them correctly in various focussed and decontextualized
exercises. In real communication, however, they usually make even basic
mistakes and their performance is far from fluent self-confident communication.
From the perspective of grammar teaching, this phenomenon seems to be caused by
the traditionally dominant focus on explicit grammar knowledge, on the
presentation of grammar structures and the overemphasizing of correct forms to
the detriment of their meaning and functions, i.e. overemphasizing accuracy
over fluency. This approach also leads to a lack of focus and classroom time
for practicing and the automization of learnt theoretical knowledge, which is
crucial for fluent and correct speech perception and production in real
communicative situations. Empirical evidence as well as some research findings
(Lojová 2004, Straková 2001, Rafajlovičová
2000.) show that one of the reasons for
this situation is the survival
of “traditional” (as opposed to innovative)
ways of teaching FL at our schools despite an increasing number of teachers who
apply modern approaches and teaching methods.
The research findings may
signal deeper problems that are rooted in the theoretical background of
communicative methodology and its application to our socio-cultural and
educational context resulting in confusion about the role of theoretical
knowledge in FL teaching and a lack of clear guidance for teachers as for
teaching grammar. The detailed theoretical analyses of the problem pointed to
the urgent necessity to search
for a model of teaching and learning FL grammar that would match specific Slovak conditions as well
as the developmental and psychological characteristics and needs of our learners
in secondary education and so increase the effectiveness of teaching English.
State-of-the-art professional
literature provides us with various conceptions of language and theories and
models of language learning. We consider the most suitable theory to be Keith
Johnson’s conception of language learning as skill development based on
Anderson’s cognitive ACT model of learning.
(Anderson 1989, Johnson 1996). It offers a natural psychological learning
theory framework for the communicative approach, which was previously lacking
despite theoretically highly elaborated and widely applied communicative
methodologies. The conception provides
us with the detailed characteristics of the central psychological concepts
within the study of cognitive skills acquisition: declarative knowledge,
procedural knowledge, and atomization. Psychologically speaking, the
distinction between declarative and procedural knowledge is the distinction in
how knowledge is represented in memory. Declarative representation (i.e. the knowledge of theoretical
rules) means that learners store knowledge in long-term memory as a database,
which takes the form of a set of semantic networks and also a general set of
interpretative procedures (rules) to use the knowledge. When
parts of the database are required to perform a certain operation, a set of
general procedures is used (learners consciously apply the learned rule).
Procedural knowledge (i.e. the ability to use the form correctly without being
aware of the explicit rule) is embedded in procedures for action and not kept
in a separate storage area. When the form is required, it is there readily to
hand – a set of specific programs that incorporate required data within them.
Automization
is a fundamental component of skill development. A newly learned skill
takes up a great deal of conscious attention, or channel capacity. The role of
automization in skill learning is to free important channel capacity for the
higher-level tasks which require it. Both ways of memory storage have their
advantages and disadvantages and Keith Johnson in his model tries to utilize
advantages of each way, as they both may be useful in different language tasks
as well as in learning under different conditions and in different
environments. Declarative representation
has generative character, it is economical as for memory capacity, there is low
risk as for an appropriate use, but it is heavy on channel capacity as it
requires conscious processes, and therefore is slow in production. Procedural knowledge,
on the other hand, is faster in production, light on channel capacity, but it
is not in generative form, uneconomical, and there is higher risk that the form
will not be used properly and adequately in new situations and contexts.
The
detailed explanation of psychological characteristics of declarative and
procedural knowledge, of their distinction and relations (see in Johnson, 1996)
enables different sequencing of methodological steps when teaching languages in
different situations, conditions and contexts. The learning sequence that seems
the most suitable to our conditions, i.e. to secondary education in Slovakia is:
Declarative encoding – procedural encoding – tuning
Declarative
encoding means that the information, i.e. an explicit grammar rule, is provided
usually through instruction. The aim is for a learner to develop the initial
declarative representation of the language structure, which is a starting point
for proceduralization.
Procedural
encoding is the movement from declarative to procedural, i.e. a change in how
knowledge is represented in memory. With practice, the knowledge is converted
into a procedural form in which it is directly applied. In that case required information is retrieved
from long-term memory and held in working memory. That is, the database
specific to the task becomes incorporated into the production. Proceduralization,
i.e. changing declarative knowledge to procedural, occurs through the process
of automization when learners gradually automate the use of consciously learnt
grammar forms so that they can free their channel capacity and concentrate
attention on more complex functions such as to convey the proper meaning,
semantic relations, the choice of adequate linguistic devices, feedback from an
interlocutor or social environment, etc. At the procedural stage three general
learning mechanisms operate: generalization, discrimination and strengthening
process. These mechanisms together constitute a process of tuning, which is
needed in comprehending and conveying meanings clearly (Johnson 1996). Tuning
is a very important long-lasting process that we tend not to be aware of and
therefore underestimate. The result of
sufficient tuning is that learners fully understand not only the form but
mainly the possible meanings and functions of a given structure in various
contexts and situations. Particularly obvious it is in the ability to differentiate
between similar structures and to use them correctly and appropriately in diverse
communicative situations, contexts and discourses. As the results of our
research show, this seems to be the key problem of secondary school learners
(Lojová 2004).
However,
when the full proceduralization has been achieved, it is necessary to
maintaining the declarative representation. There is a tendency for declarative
representations to fall away when procedural knowledge is developed. It means
that when learners use a structure automatically for a longer time, they tend
to forget the initial declarative rule. This should be avoided, as declarative
representation always remains important, particularly in some special writing
tasks or when full proceduralization has not been achieved.
Comparing Johnson’s
model with traditional teaching (based on grammar-translation and audio-lingual
methods) points to crucial differences in emphasis and the proportion of time
devoted to theoretical knowledge and practice, which may be illustrated as
follows:
Traditional
teaching
DEC focussed practice procederal.
Johnson‘s model
The model also underlines different approaches to
practice, providing a thorough specification of how automization takes place
and how it can be facilitated so as to result in full proceduralization. The full understanding of
the above described characteristics and processes enable educators to sequence
the teaching/learning stages according to their learners´
characteristics, current conditions or different contexts. The described
sequence of learning stages is recommended for teaching FL in secondary education
and to adult learners. For younger learners different sequence should be adopted,
based more on the initial procedural stage. This is due to developmental
differences, in particular the level of cognitive development, which is fundamental
for the comprehension of abstract linguistic knowledge.
Theoretically speaking, Johnson’s conception
reformulates the aims
of communicative methodology as a shift from “message focus” to “form defocus”, which explicitly defines the place of declarative
grammar knowledge in communicative teaching. In addition, it justifies and underlines
the importance of meaningful practice
for the development of communicative competence. The analysis of
automization of language forms emphasizes its central place and points to an
urgent need to focus much more on appropriate teaching and learning activities.
Another important advantage of implementing Johnson’s theory to FL teaching in Slovakia is, that it respects our educational
traditions which have lead to a relatively high level of metalinguistic
awareness as a consequence of the ways of teaching the Slovak language in our
schools.
To provide educators with an effective tool for
developing FL communicative competence, the theoretical analysis and adaptation
of the model to our conditions must be followed by detailed instructions for
its didactic application (see in Lojova 2004). They can be summarised briefly as
follows:
As for declarative knowledge, teachers may use any
methods and techniques to present information and assist learners to consciously create
a comprehensible inner representation of a FL grammar system,
which is
a starting point for proceduralization. In so
doing they should bear in mind some basic rules:
o
Teachers should not overestimate the importance of
theoretical knowledge as it tends to create communication barriers;
o
Fundamental is the choice and sequencing of theoretical knowledge, which is
normally given in a textbook. However, a teacher should be flexible and adapt
the content to learners;
o
Learners should learn rules relevant for their
communicative needs, otherwise they cannot practice them meaningfully and effectively;
o
Learners should not be
overloaded at a given time, to avoid
creating chaos in their system of grammar knowledge.
o
New rules must be connected with the learner's
existing system so that permanent
restructuring may occur.
As for automization, the crucial is for a teacher to facilitate it and to
assist learners so that they can achieve full proceduralization. In so doing it
is important to bear in mind that automization is a long-lasting process, which
needs time and teacher´s conscious control. It is also
a gradual process beginning with the automization of lower subskills that are
components of higher-level skills. During this process learnt
knowledge needs time for maturation, which must be respected also in the
treatment of errors. It is obvious that in any learning activity more
structures are automatized at a time. Their different stages of automization
should be carefully monitored and systematically coordinated by the teacher,
which seems to be the most difficult task.
It is obvious that automization may be facilitated
mainly through repetition and “never-ending” practice. There is an urgent need
in Slovakia to adopt appropriate teaching and learning activities that
facilitate the automization of language forms much more than we have been doing
so far. When organizing teaching activities and selecting appropriate
techniques, teachers should be aware of some fundamental principles derived
from psychology of learning that underlie the effective practice with
long-lasting effect:
o
It is better to thoroughly practice less than
superficially more.
o
It is better to develop in learners self-confident
communication at a lower level than to overload them with too many rules, which
may create communicative barriers, inhibitions, demotivation and frustration
with their ability to use all the rules correctly and fluently.
o
Newly practiced knowledge must be permanently
connected with the existing system of
knowledge. As a result of the cyclical
practice of learnt structures, learners will comprehend differences in the
usage, meaning, and function of similar structures and gradually use them correctly
and fluently in communication.
o
For the automization of lower subskills various
drills, decontextualized and focussed activities may be effectively used.
Learners, however, should not spend too much time on these activities.
o
A short period of focussed activities should be
followed by extensive practice in communicative and productive activities.
o
Real and meaningful communication tasks must be
perceived by students as an opportunity to communicate and express themselves,
not as an opportunity to practice and revise grammar forms.
o
Practice must also be based on the principles of
task-based learning in
which the tasks are sequenced according to their level of difficulty. A teacher
can control the gradation of task difficulty in
many ways (see in Skehan 1998b, Willis 1996 et al).
o
To make automization more
effective, it is vital to promote much more student-centred teaching, an
approach that teachers claim to favour though its application is disproved by
the research findings as well as by everyday experience.
Obviously, in Slovakia, where learners in their
everyday life are not immersed in FL environment where proceduralization could
be enhanced, it is not possible to achieve full automization of all grammar
structures covered in secondary school syllabus. To make FL teaching more
effective and to lay sound foundation for life-long learning and
proceduralization, it is necessary to differenciate which structures should be
fully proceduralized, which just partially and which can achieve just the
declarative stage (see in Lojová 2004).
I am aware of the fact that some of these principles provoke
discussion and are not compatible with current tendencies in FL teaching aiming
at standardized achievements. However, when looking for the ways of achieving standards
and general objectives we cannot be too content centred but we have to keep in
mind learners with their developmental and individual differences, internal,
environmental, social conditions and constraints (Lojová 2005, Tudor
1996). The implementation of the adapted
model is a gradual process consisting of some crucial steps and various tasks.
Among these the fundamental seem to be the re-evaluation of curricula,
preparation of teaching materials, and approppriate teacher training.
Résumé
In Slovakia empirical evidence and research findings point
to the necessity to search for a new model of teaching foreign languages
that would explicitly define the place of
declarative and procedural knowledge. Johnson’s conception of language
learning as skill development seems to be the most suitable model. The article
focuses on the explanation of its basic concepts as well as on its adaptation
to our current conditions in secondary education. The theoretical analysis is
followed by some suggestions for the successful implementation of the model
into teaching practice.
Anderson, J. R. 1989.
Practice, Working Memory, and the ACT Theory of Skill Acquisition. In: Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 1989, vol.15, no.3, pp.527 - 530
De Keyser, R. M. 1997. Beyond Explicit Rule
Learning. In: Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1997, vol.19, no.2, pp. 195 - 221
Ellis, R. 1997. Second
Language Acquisition Research and Language Teaching. Oxford: Oxford
University Press, 1997
JOHNSON, K.
1996. Language Teaching and Skill Development. Oxford: Blackwell
Publishers, 1996
LOJOVÁ,
G. 2004. Teória a prax
vyučovania gramatiky cudzích jazykov. Bratislava: Monitor
Promotion, 2004,.
Lojova, G. 2005. Individuálne
osobitosti pri učení sa cudzích jazykov. Bratislava:
Univerzita Ko menského.
MACWINNING, B.
1997. Implicit and Explicit Processes. In:Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
1997, vol.19, no.3, pp. 277
- 281
Rafajlovičová, R. 2000. Testing verb forms through discrete-point and open-ended tasks. Prešov: PU FHaPV,
RUTHERFORD, W.
and SHARWOOD SMITH, M. 1988. Grammar and Second Language Teaching.
Boston: Heinle and Heinle
Publishers. 1988.
SCHMIDT, R.
1992. Psychological Mechanisms Underlying Second Language Fluency. In: Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
1992, vol.14, no. 4, pp.357 - 385
SKEHAN,
P. 1998a. .A Cognitive Approach to Language
Learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1998, ISBN 0 19 4372170
SKEHAN, P.
1998b. Task based instruction. In
Grabe 1998.
Straková, Z. 2001. Učebnica v teórii a praxi. Prešov:
Metodické centrum,.
TUDOR, I. 1996.
Learner
– Centeredness as Language Education. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press, 1996
WILLIS, J.
1996. A Framework for Task-Based Learning. London: Longman.