Филологические науки/ 7. Язык, речь, речевая коммуникация
Бобошко А. М., Бобошко Ю. М.
Винницкий государственный педагогический университет,
Украина
The
concept as it is represented in modern linguistics
The term “concept” is used in mainstream cognitive science and
philosophy of mind. A concept is an abstract idea or a mental symbol, typically
associated with a corresponding representation in a language or symbology [2;
64].
Today linguists agree that:
1) concepts are abstract in that they omit the differences of the things
in their extension, treating the members of the extension as if they were
identical. Classical concepts are universal in that they apply equally to
everything in their extension;
2) concepts are also the basic elements of propositions, much the same way
a word is the basic semantic element of a sentence. Unlike perceptions, which
are particular images of individual objects, concepts cannot be visualized [3;
68];
3) concepts are expected to be useful in dealing with reality. Generally
speaking, concepts are taken to be (a) acquired dispositions to recognize
perceived objects as being of this kind or of that ontological kind, and at the
same time (b) to understand what this kind or that kind of object is like, and
consequently (c) to perceive a number of perceived particulars as being the
same in kind and to discriminate between them and other sensible particulars
that are different in kind [8; 34-37];
4) concepts are bearers of meaning, as opposed to agents of meaning. A
single concept can be expressed by any number of languages. The fact that
concepts are in some sense independent of language makes translation possible -
words in various languages have identical meaning, because they express one and
the same concept.
Several partly or fully distinct concepts may share the same term. These
different concepts are easily confused by mistakenly being used
interchangeably, which is a fallacy. Also, the notions of term and concept are
often confused, although the two are not the same [6; 44].
Although the mainstream literature in cognitive science regards the
concept as a kind of mental particular, it has been suggested by some theorists
that concepts are real things.
Traditionally, content analysis has most often been thought of in terms
of conceptual analysis. In conceptual analysis, a concept is chosen for
examination, and the analysis involves quantifying and tallying its presence.
On analyzing main approaches in the sphere of concept understanding,
let's briefly introduce today's views on conceptual analysis. It begins with
identifying research questions and choosing a sample or samples. Once chosen,
the text must be coded into manageable content categories. The process of
coding is basically one of selective reduction. By reducing the text to
categories consisting of a word, set of words or phrases, the researcher can
focus on, and code for, specific words or patterns that are indicative of the
research question [4;
81].
The focus of study linguistic concept in use is oriented on how our
mental representations guide our linguistic output. Thus, linguists are
interested in how humans structure their experience and represent information
about the world, for instance, about the orientation of objects in a room or an
agent’s movement through space, and the linguistic consequences of such
representations. Experimental psychology provides us with a rich body of data
on how humans may store spatial knowledge. For instance, a large body of
research shows that one such representation of spatial knowledge may be as a
mental model [5;
91-92].
Variability in linguistic strategies has been investigated in great
detail in register theory, in which it is held that the situation has a great
impact on the speakers’ linguistic choices. However, what we can observe when
we look at situated talk is that the speakers’ concepts of the situation may be
very different, and that these differences are strongly reflected in the
participants’ linguistic behaviour. What a situation consists in is therefore
also determined by the speakers’ mental representation of it. Thus, the problem
is not only that we find a diversity of possible representations, but there may
also be a variety of strategies of using them, depending on the speakers’
conceptualization of what the situation consists in. We therefore need to look
at actual situations and investigate the concepts in use [1; 152].
In cognitive linguistics, the relationship between linguistic structure
and general cognitive structures also constitutes the focus of linguistic
investigation. Cognitive linguistics however usually does not consider the
conditions of usage of the cognitive structures as they are encoded in linguistic
structures, and may, for instance, only provide lists of possible cognitive
metaphors. For example, metaphorical concepts of the same phenomenon may
sometimes have opposite orientations. Cognitive linguistic analysis reveals the
different underlying concepts, but usually does not show the conditions of
variability for the different orientations [7; 23].
ЛИТЕРАТУРА
1.
Арутюнова Н.Д.
Язык и мир человека / Н.Д. Арутюнова. – [2-е изд., испр.]. – М.: Школа “Языки
русской культуры”, 1999. – I-XV. – 896
с.
2.
Булыгина Т.В.,
Шмелев А.Д. Языковая концептуализация мира / Т.В.Булыгина, А.Д.Шмелев. – М.:
Школа “Языки русской культуры”, 1997. – 576 с.
3.
Валгина Н.С.
Теория текста: [учебное пособие] / Н.С. Валгина. – М.: Логос, 2003. – 280 с.
4.
Пеляева
Н.Ф. Лингвистический анализ художественного текста / Н.Ф. Пеляева – Л.:
Просвещение, 1980. – 271 с.
5. Dijk Tuen van. Episodes as units of discourse analysis /
[Ed. D. Tannen] // Analyzing Discourse:
Text and Talk. –
Washington DC: Georgetown University Press, 1982. – 143 p.
6. Kintsch
W. Reading comprehension as a function
of text structure / [A. S. Reber, D. L. Scarborough, Eds.]
// Toward a psychology of reading. - New York: Wiley, 1977.
– 428 p.
7. Peer W.
Stylistics and Psychology: Investigation of Foregrounding / W. Peer. - L.: Croom Helm, 1986. – 211
p.